Vaia - The all-in-one study app.
4.8 • +11k Ratings
More than 3 Million Downloads
Free
Americas
Europe
Many psychologists have studied moral reasoning in humans, as it is one of the central elements of judgement and justice in today's society. Our morals, or societal moral norms, dictate what is right and wrong and, therefore, what we deem acceptable. By understanding theories on levels of moral reasoning and cognitive distortions, we can identify the origins of delinquent behaviour and the logic behind such decisions.
Explore our app and discover over 50 million learning materials for free.
Lerne mit deinen Freunden und bleibe auf dem richtigen Kurs mit deinen persönlichen Lernstatistiken
Jetzt kostenlos anmeldenMany psychologists have studied moral reasoning in humans, as it is one of the central elements of judgement and justice in today's society. Our morals, or societal moral norms, dictate what is right and wrong and, therefore, what we deem acceptable. By understanding theories on levels of moral reasoning and cognitive distortions, we can identify the origins of delinquent behaviour and the logic behind such decisions.
Fig. 1 - Cognitive distortions and levels of moral reasoning are cognitive explanations of crime.
Cognitive distortions are abnormal behaviours or thoughts that alter a person's perception of reality. Naturally, this can directly affect behaviour and how people interpret situations. Cognitive distortions are irrational thoughts that cause you to perceive reality negatively, even though reality does not reflect this.
John Gibbs et al. (1995) established four types of cognitive distortions common in criminals:
Cognitive distortions go hand in hand with moral reasoning, especially concerning criminal behaviour, as they cause potentially abnormal thought processes and behaviours.Jean Piaget studied the cognitive abilities children develop to make moral judgments. Later, Kohlberg and James Rest continued to study moral reasoning abilities in adults. Kohlberg was the first researcher to apply moral judgement to criminal behaviour.
Moral reasoning is the cognitive process of reasoning whilst considering ethical implications, also known as right and wrong. The perspective recognises right and wrong and acknowledges the rules of social conduct.
Cognitive distortions may explain the moral reasoning behind some offences. Gibbs et al. (1995) identified cognitive distortions of particular importance in offending behaviours. These irrational distortions are problematic because they affect thoughts and behaviour because the distorted view of reality leads to abnormal behaviour.
Let's explore the different types of cognitive distortions further.
In the context of criminal behaviour, the hostile attribution of bias is related to the offender’s misinterpretation of other people’s attitudes by typically assuming the worst. It is assumed that others will behave aggressively and confrontationally toward the perpetrator when this is not the case. This assumption often leads to disproportionate and aggressive responses.
Michael Schonenberg and Aiste Jusyte (2014) conducted a study in which they presented 55 violent offenders with pictures of emotionally ambiguous facial expressions and compared them to a nonaggressive control group.
They found that violent offenders were more likely to recognise anger and hostility in the images than non-violent offenders.
Kenneth Dodge and Cynthia Frame (1982) believed that the origin of these cognitive biases might stem from childhood. They conducted a study showing children a video clip of ambiguous, provocative situations in which the behaviour was neither hostile nor random. Before the study, the children who were classified as aggressive perceived the video clip as more aggressive overall than those who were classified as nonaggressive.
Research shows that sex offenders try to minimise their guilt and sometimes even shift the blame to the victims.
Minimalisation is a type of cognitive distortion. The person tries to minimise or reduce the severity of their crimes and behaviours. In other words, it can be seen as denial or self-deception. Minimising the seriousness of the situation emphasises a refusal to accept what one has done. The consequences are exaggerated (either under or over-exaggerated) to some degree.
An example of minimalisation is if someone were to steal from a shop and then describe their criminal act as a job. They would justify their misconduct by saying that it is their family’s source of income, and they had to do it to provide for their family. Doing so is a way of downplaying the seriousness of the crime.
Kennedy and Grubin (1992) found most convicted sex offenders tend to blame the victim for the crime. Another quarter believed that it had ended positively for the victim. Some thought no harm was done to the victim (especially the case of offenders who had sexually assaulted children).
Howard Barbaree (1991) found that of 26 incarcerated rapists, 54% completely denied the crime. The other 40% justified the harm they inflicted on victims by downplaying it.
Kohlberg agreed with Piaget’s initial interpretation of children’s moral development and wanted to expand on these points. This led to Kohlberg’s stages of moral development, detailing three levels of reasoning comprised of six overall stages. Kohlberg's stages of moral development occur around the following ages:
Most people tend to end up in level two, and rarely do people reach level three. Kohlberg developed and investigated his stages of moral development theory through ten moral stories presented to participants. One famous example is the Heinz Dilemma.
The Heinz Dilemma details a moral dilemma where participants had to decide if Heinz's actions were justified based on the story.
Heinz had a sick wife and needed expensive medication to save her. Unfortunately, he could not afford it, so broke into the shop with the expensive medication to steal it to help save his dying wife.
Kohlberg asked participants if the actions were justified and the reasoning behind the participant's decisions.
Fig. 2 - Kohlberg described different stages of moral reasonming¹.
Preconventional reasoning is the first stage of moral development in which the sense of moral values is externally directed.
Children do not yet have a personal moral code at this stage because they have not internalised social conventions about right and wrong. Instead, adult norms and the consequences of breaking their rules guide moral decisions.
Children understand rules based on how authority figures enact rewards or punishments. Actions associated with ‘offending behaviour’ or breaking rules at this stage are:
If my actions result in punishment, it must have been a wrong decision.
If my actions lead to a reward, it must be the right decision.
Children’s best interest determines the proper behaviour at this stage, so they show little interest in others and are concerned with their gain. Actions associated with ‘offending behaviour’ or breaking rules at this stage are:
What are the gains of breaking the rules?
The offence is likely to occur if the potential gain is worth it.
Conventional reasoning is the second stage of moral development. The sense of morality at the conventional level is tied to personal and social Relationships.
Children and adolescents accept the rules of authority figures because they provide positive Relationships and maintain social order.
It is the stage when people begin to internalise the moral standards of adult roles in society. Attachment to rules and conventions is more rigid, and the rule’s relevance or integrity is rarely questioned. At this stage, social rules are essential to ensure the functioning of society. However, this affects our moral views of what is right and wrong.
In this stage, the emphasis is on positive behaviour and a good and balanced relationship with others. The individual tends to be good so that others perceive them as such.
Therefore, this positive behaviour is strongly associated with others’ recognition. Children need the approval of peers and authority figures and will behave accordingly to avoid being disliked.
At this stage, the child accepts rules and conventions due to the importance of maintaining a functioning society. The laws are seen as equal to everyone, and obeying rules is considered valuable and essential.
Moral reasoning is about the need for individual approval, like in the previous stage. There is a duty to support laws and rules.
Most people in society remain at stage four, where an outside force dictates morality.
Postconventional morality is the third level of moral development. At this level, people develop their own ethical and moral principles. At this stage, the individual understands some laws are unjust and should be changed or abolished, characterised by a growing awareness.
These may be abstract ethical principles and values, usually aimed at the greater good of humanity.
Postconventional moralists have their ethical principles. These principles usually rely on ensuring the welfare of the most significant number of people possible and prioritising human rights. Postconventionalists place their moral evaluation of a situation above social conventions. Some theorists assume that many people will not reach such a level of abstract moral reasoning.
Kohlberg believed this stage was unattainable for most, where the world is diverse with different cultures, values, rights, and opinions. Perspectives at this level should be mutual and respected as unique to each.
Individuals see laws not as rigid edicts but as social agreements. People at this stage should allow and encourage changes in cooperation for the highest good of the general welfare.
The five reasons are the theoretical principles of democratic government.
Moral reasoning relies on the abstract application of universal ethical principles at this stage. Usually, these principles focus on moral values such as equality, dignity, or respect.
At this stage, people believe laws are valid only if they rely on justice. They are aware that commitment to justice entails not obeying unjust laws. People who choose ethical principles want to follow those laws.
When they violate them, they feel remorse. They select certain attitudes because they believe it is in their best interest. Kohlberg believed that at this stage, it is difficult to identify individuals who consistently act at this level.
Ma (2013) found the first four stages nearly the same across populations, but stages five and six were scarce.
Kohlberg was the first researcher to apply moral reasoning to criminal behaviour and proposed three levels of moral reasoning. The higher the level, the more complex the stages of moral development become.
Moral reasoning development is a product of cognitive development. We can see how cognitive development and moral reasoning tie in together by understanding how criminals develop different moral outlooks.
It is theorised that criminals do not progress through the stages of moral reasoning and cognitive development milestones like other members of society. Instead, they remain at lower levels of moral reasoning (Allen et al., 2001).
The preconventional stage links to the need to avoid punishment and receive rewards. It is also associated with less mature reasonings. People classified at this level are more likely to commit a crime if they can avoid responsibility or punishment due to their actions or if they can be rewarded due to their misbehaviour.
Understanding the strengths and weaknesses of the approaches discussed above is essential.
Research supports the cognitive explanation for offending behaviour. Palmer and Hollin (1998) compared the moral reasoning of female and male nonoffenders and offenders.
They found the offender group exhibited lower moral reasoning maturity than the nonoffender group, which was the same for both genders. Female nonoffenders showed higher moral judgement than male nonoffenders.
Crick and Dodge (1994) found a relationship between hostile attribution bias and aggression observed in children and adolescents. The link between actual situations is considered one of the antecedents of aggressive behaviour in children, adolescents, and adults, possibly leading to criminal behaviour.
Another strength of the cognitive explanation of delinquent behaviour is that it is widely applicable.
For example, understanding cognitive distortions have been shown to be beneficial in treating criminal behaviour, particularly in rehabilitating sex offenders for whom cognitive behavioural therapy is used.
The rehabilitation included cognitive behavioural therapy methods that directly address dysfunctional thoughts (cognitive distortions). They learn how to gain a less distorted view of their actions by confronting their crime. Studies found less denial and minimisation in therapy are highly correlated with a lower risk of recidivism, i.e., reoffending.
Fig, 3 - Rehabilitation includes methods such as cognitive behavioural therapy.
Minimalisation can be seen as descriptive rather than explanatory. It describes how an offender distorts the reality of their offending behaviour but does not explain the reasons for committing the crime. However, minimalisation could predict reoffending.
The cognitive explanation for delinquent behaviour using the moral reasoning model is gender-specific because it was conducted on American males.
This is problematic because the model does not represent a ‘natural’ maturation stage of cognitive development. Alternatively, Gibbs proposed two levels of moral reasoning: the mature and the immature. These levels tend to be more general when applied to different cultures. It questions the credibility of the cognitive explanation of offending behaviour.
According to Kohlberg, the levels of moral reasoning are:
Neural explanations suggest someone’s moral reasoning for crime is linked to neurological factors.
An example of someone with cognitive distortions is when they assume that someone looking at them is doing so to pick a fight; they were aggressive and did not just happen to make eye contact. Similarly, a person can construe a smile as aggressive.
An example of a level in moral reasoning is preconventional reasoning level (level one). Children understand rules based on how authority figures enact rewards or punishments in this stage.
Kohlberg believed this stage was unattainable for most, where the world is diverse with different cultures, values, rights, and opinions. Perspectives at this level should be mutual and respected as unique to each.
Individuals see laws not as rigid edicts but as social agreements. People at this stage should allow and encourage changes in cooperation for the highest good of the general welfare.
Moral reasoning development is a product of cognitive development.
Flashcards in Level of Moral Reasoning and Cognitive Distortions14
Start learningWhat is the level of morals?
The level of morals regards the perspective on perceiving right or wrong.
What is cognitive distortion?
A cognitive distortion is an irrational thought, you perceive reality in a negative way, but in fact, everything is going well.
What is the level of moral reasoning behind criminal behaviour?
The connection between cognitive distortion and criminal behaviour is explained as faulty thinking processed in the offender’s mind. This is the level of moral reasoning behind this type of criminal behaviour.
What are the three levels of moral reasoning Kohlberg proposed?
Kohlberg proposed three levels of moral reasoning, preconventional, conventional, and postconventional.
What is one example of cognitive distortion?
One example of cognitive distortions is the hostile attribution to bias, which is associated with misinterpreting other people’s actions or assuming others are aggressive and confrontational when they are not.
What is minimalisation?
Minimisation is an example of cognitive distortion, defined as the attempt of denying or minimising serious offences.
Already have an account? Log in
The first learning app that truly has everything you need to ace your exams in one place
Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.
Save explanations to your personalised space and access them anytime, anywhere!
Sign up with Email Sign up with AppleBy signing up, you agree to the Terms and Conditions and the Privacy Policy of Vaia.
Already have an account? Log in