StudySmarter - The all-in-one study app.
4.8 • +11k Ratings
More than 3 Million Downloads
Free
Americas
Europe
Imagine your friends are over at your place, and your brother shows some embarrassing home video of you from when you were a kid. Would you rather have your brother show the video or tell the story in this situation? If you're well aware that memory is reconstructive, your best bet may probably be the second option.First, we will look…
Explore our app and discover over 50 million learning materials for free.
Save the explanation now and read when you’ve got time to spare.
SaveLerne mit deinen Freunden und bleibe auf dem richtigen Kurs mit deinen persönlichen Lernstatistiken
Jetzt kostenlos anmeldenImagine your friends are over at your place, and your brother shows some embarrassing home video of you from when you were a kid. Would you rather have your brother show the video or tell the story in this situation? If you're well aware that memory is reconstructive, your best bet may probably be the second option.
According to Frederic Charles Bartlett (1932), memories are reconstructive. This means we don't store and play back memories like a tape recorder. Our memory undergoes structural changes over time, leading to forgetting details and inaccuracies upon retrieval. Our prior knowledge or schema assists memory retrieval by filling in the gaps.
Schemas are pre-existing mental representations or expectations of something based on our past knowledge.
Reconstructive memory in psychology refers to the retrieval of memory, which involves filling in memory gaps by using prior knowledge, experience, reasoning, and intended outcomes.
Let's look at how Bartlett demonstrated memory reconstruction in his 'War of the Ghosts' study.
He asked students from Cambridge University to read a famous North American folktale entitled 'War of the Ghosts.' He presented a story from a different cultural background to see whether the participants' schema would affect the story recollection.
The results showed that the students recalled the story based on their cultural expectations, which Bartlett called rationalisation. Participants left out some details, such as those specific to North American culture. Bartlett concluded that schemas could impact memory, such as when recalling stories that conflict with your prior knowledge (e.g., different culture).
With the introduction of memory reconstruction, psychology learned what influences memory retrieval, such as prior knowledge and other cognitive processes. Here are some examples of significant memory reconstruction research:
Fig. 1 Car collision
Loftus and Palmer (1974) investigated whether verbs (e.g., smashed and contacted) used in a question can influence memory. Participants watched a vehicle accident video and estimated car speeds before the collision. When the question included “smashed,” participants reported a broken glass (even though there was none) and that the car traveled 41 mph. On the other hand, when the verb “contacted” was part of the question, people said the car was going 32 mph.
They concluded that when we recall a memory, we also try to access and reconstruct it based on knowledge from other occurrences since then, which can alter our memory.
In another study of memory reconstruction, Dooling and Christiaansen (1977) had people read a passage about a woman named Carol Harris. According to the text, Carol Harris was an uncontrollable child, even at the tender age of eight, leading her parents to believe that they needed to seek professional treatment. Her parents invested in private tutoring for her. Researchers examined participants' passage memory after a week. Before the test, the researchers told half the participants that the passage was about Helen Keller.
They found that people who “knew” the text was about Helen Keller remembered reading that the girl was deaf, even though it's not in the passage. Hearing about Helen Keller activated their prior knowledge about her, which led them to remember something that wasn't in the passage.
While these studies demonstrate how prior knowledge can cause errors in our memory, interestingly, two other researchers, namely Steyvers and Hemmer (2012), argued the opposite, that prior knowledge could improve memory in everyday situations. As you will read in the later section, you will see how Steyvers and Hemmer demonstrated reconstruction from memory in naturalistic environments.
Earlier, we've seen lab evidence of the reconstructive nature of memory. But before we better understand reconstruction from memory in naturalistic environments, let's look at what happens during memory retrieval and the reconstructive memory model.
If you lost your keys, naturally, you would try to remember where you last put them. Is it in your pocket? Or on the kitchen counter? These are cues we use when retrieving a target memory.
Retrieval is to trace back a memory (e.g., concept, event, or experience) assisted by cues to bring that memory into awareness.
As you try to access a particular memory, your mind begins to search for cues about the event. These cues help you retrieve your target memory through associations.
For example, suppose someone asks you about the first psychologist who comes to mind when you hear cognitive psychology. You might say Jean Piaget. Piaget then represents a strong association with cognitive psychology.
Memory retrieval involves cues and associations that lead you to the target memory. It's also important to note that cues are anything that may well reflect the content of the target memory we're trying to access.
For example, seeing a photo of the lead actor may remind you of the movie you're trying to recall, or hearing the movie's soundtrack can serve as a cue to access the target memory.
In what specific way do cues and associations help in memory retrieval? One way is through spreading activation. Cues “spread energy” to other memories associated with the target memory. The higher the activation memory trace receives from a cue, the more likely one can access and retrieve the memory.
Activation level is the fluctuating internal condition of memory that influences its accessibility.
The activation level increases when a cue is physically present (e.g., seeing a photo) or when we try to think about the memory trace (e.g., thinking about the movie).
Earlier, we saw how prior knowledge fills in the memory gaps as we reconstruct memories. Now let's look further at how the reconstructive memory model explains memory reconstruction.
The reconstructive memory model states that for every experience we have, we store its memory in the long-term memory (LTM) as separate pieces of information linked to each other in different ways. We also store a script that we use to recall the memory.
This information, along with the script, makes up the memory of the event. We glean information from these components when attempting to recall an experience. If we have a clear idea of what we believe happened (a script), we may omit or change details so that they conform to the script. Furthermore, recalled facts could potentially be incorrect if the script from memory is incorrect.
The reconstructive memory model also suggests that we don't store everything as correctly in our memory when the event occurred, leading us to retrieve similar memories when prompted to recollect a particular memory.
When we recall ordering burgers and fries at a specific restaurant, we may have derived this information from past knowledge that we usually order this food whenever we eat outside.
This process creates new mental representations of the incident, which we might use to retell the recollection.
Earlier, we mentioned Bartlett's previous memory research, which showed prior knowledge affects our memory. However, this model focused mainly on memory errors due to prior knowledge.
In particular, lab studies of this nature have aimed to draw out false memories. This is because the method often withholds something from what the participants need to remember, such as the item they expect to see the most. This means that studies like this one had low ecological validity. The conclusion was that prior knowledge changes episodic memories.
Fig. 2 Naturalistic office environment
Suppose participants viewed an office scene in which they expected to see books. However, there are no books shown. Then, researchers ask the participants to recall the scene. Many of them would recall seeing books as they are part of the schema of an office.
The conclusion was that prior knowledge changes episodic memories. However, Steyvers & Hemmer (2012) theorised that prior knowledge could help save and enhance memories. This is because, based on what we already know, we would expect to find certain things in a scene, e.g., a computer in an office.
In lab studies of memories, stimuli usually are manipulated, like in our example of an office scene with no books. Studies like this don't reflect a naturalistic environment. Therefore, Steyvers & Hemmer (2012) theorised that if there are experiments aimed at finding the effect of prior knowledge on memory in naturalistic environments, the result would be positive rather than negative.
To investigate how prior knowledge affects the reconstruction from memory in naturalistic environments—the interaction between episodic memories and prior knowledge. All participants were from the University of California. The researchers divided them into three groups (expectation test, perception test, and experimental memory condition).
Expectation Test: 22 participants named objects they would expect to find in five different naturalistic settings: office, kitchen, hotel, urban setting, and dining room. They had to name the objects by typing them on a screen and had one minute for each scene. The researchers measured the frequency of all objects participants had named, e.g., all 22 participants expected to see a television in a hotel room.
Fig. 3 Naturalistic hotel room
Perception Test: Another group of 22 participants looked at 25 images of these five naturalistic scenes. They had to recall all the objects they had seen. The researchers presented the objects according to their recall frequency (most and least recalled).
Forty-nine participants who hadn't been part of either initial test were randomly selected. From the perception test in the initial testing, the researchers took ten images — two from each of the five scenes (office, kitchen, hotel, urban setting, and dining room). These were the two that had the highest recall frequency.
The researchers split participants into two groups. Group 1 looked at the images for two seconds each. The hypothesis was that the participants would not remember all the details due to the short time. Because of this, they would rely on semantic knowledge when recalling objects in the scenes. Group 2 looked at five images for ten seconds each. This was an episodic memory test, as these participants had more time to look at the images. The participants recalled the objects they saw in the images.
In the two-second condition, the participants correctly recalled an average of 7.75 objects. In the ten-second condition, the score was higher — 10.05 objects. For both situations, the average accuracy was 90 percent. The researchers also examined how often the participants named objects that weren't in the scenes. Incorrect recall of high-probability objects was nine percent (probability calculation conducted from the initial tests). Incorrect recall of low-probability objects was 18 percent.
Fig. 4 Naturalistic urban setting
According to this model, semantic knowledge can help accurately recall episodic memories of naturalistic settings. This is beneficial as more cognitive resources can be used to attend to other parts of the scene, such as novel items.
Steyvers and Hemmer concluded that semantic knowledge can help accurately recall episodic memories of naturalistic settings.
This is beneficial as more cognitive resources are available and can be used to attend to other parts of the scene, such as novel items.
The reconstructive memory theory suggests that we store memories as separate, linked pieces of information and a script we use to narrate the memory. We don't record everything accurately in our memory when the event occurs, leading us to draw information from similar experiences upon recollection.
Bartlett is associated with this view. He conducted a seminal study in 1932 called War of the Ghosts.
Yes, episodic memory is reconstructive because prior knowledge affects our memories.
This is because schemas change memories to fit our expectations of certain things and situations. Research has found that prior knowledge changes and alters memories to fit our schemas and make more sense to us (e.g. Bartlett, 1932).
Barlett (1932) theorised that we don't store and play back memories like a tape recorder. Our memory undergoes structural changes over time, leading to forgetting details and inaccuracies upon retrieval. Our prior knowledge or schema assists memory retrieval by filling in the gaps.
of the users don't pass the Reconstruction From Memory in Naturalistic Environments quiz! Will you pass the quiz?
Start QuizHow would you like to learn this content?
How would you like to learn this content?
Free psychology cheat sheet!
Everything you need to know on . A perfect summary so you can easily remember everything.
Be perfectly prepared on time with an individual plan.
Test your knowledge with gamified quizzes.
Create and find flashcards in record time.
Create beautiful notes faster than ever before.
Have all your study materials in one place.
Upload unlimited documents and save them online.
Identify your study strength and weaknesses.
Set individual study goals and earn points reaching them.
Stop procrastinating with our study reminders.
Earn points, unlock badges and level up while studying.
Create flashcards in notes completely automatically.
Create the most beautiful study materials using our templates.
Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.
Save explanations to your personalised space and access them anytime, anywhere!
Sign up with Email Sign up with AppleBy signing up, you agree to the Terms and Conditions and the Privacy Policy of StudySmarter.
Already have an account? Log in