StudySmarter - The all-in-one study app.
4.8 • +11k Ratings
More than 3 Million Downloads
Free
Americas
Europe
Have you ever walked into a house or shop and instantly started remembering the fun times you spent there? Maybe memories start coming back as you walk around and see specific items. Your memories could be happy or sad, and the location could be as small as a car or as big as a city! Your environment triggers your long-term…
Explore our app and discover over 50 million learning materials for free.
Save the explanation now and read when you’ve got time to spare.
SaveLerne mit deinen Freunden und bleibe auf dem richtigen Kurs mit deinen persönlichen Lernstatistiken
Jetzt kostenlos anmeldenHave you ever walked into a house or shop and instantly started remembering the fun times you spent there? Maybe memories start coming back as you walk around and see specific items. Your memories could be happy or sad, and the location could be as small as a car or as big as a city! Your environment triggers your long-term memory to release information into your conscious mind. Learning more about cue-dependent forgetting would be best to understand why this happens.
In cognitive psychology, there are five main theories of what causes us to forget some of our memories, and one of these is the cue dependency theory of forgetting.
Tulving proposed the theory, and he attempted to explain what happens when the right cue/ stimuli are not present when trying to remember some memories.
This is only for some memories, sometimes we can remember things without the presence of cues, these memories are likely to have a stronger impact on us, or they have been elaborately rehearsed,
Before we get into the details, let’s figure out what cue context-dependent forgetting means.
Cue-dependent forgetting, also known as memory retrieval failure, is when an individual fails to recall a memory because cues/ stimuli that were present when the memory was encoded are not present.
Much research has been conducted to investigate how retrieval failure can explain forgetting. In the following article, we will look at Godden and Baddeley’s (1975) ’s study on cue-dependent forgetting.
Godden and Baddeley (1975) conducted experimental research to highlight the importance of environmental settings in memory retrieval.
Fig. 1. Photos are an example of a memory cue.
Eighteen deep-sea divers were invited to participate in the study.
The participants were asked to memorise a list of 36 unrelated words of two or three syllables. And participants were split into two groups. One group learned the words on the beach and the second group learned the words underwater.
For the analysis:
The study used an independent measures design as participants participated in different experimental conditions.
The study used an experimental design:
The results of the study are summarised in the table below.
Table 1. Shows the average number of words recalled in the four experimental groups. | ||
---|---|---|
Learning on land | Learning underwater | |
Recall on land | 13.5 | 8.4 |
Recall underwater | 8.6 | 11.4 |
Overall:
Godden and Baddeley (1975) concluded that the mismatch between the external cues available at learning and recall led to retrieval failure. This study demonstrates context-dependent forgetting as the difficulty of recall increases due to the lack of external cues in the setting (context).
In the following, we will show you the evaluation of Godden and Baddeley’s (1975) study. This discusses real-world applications, circularity, ecological validity and ignoring other kinds of forgetting.
The understanding of how environmental context affects memory retrieval has significant real-world applications.
For example, Smith (1979) showed that just thinking of the room in which the original learning took place was as effective as actually being in the same place at the time of retrieval.
The theory has been helpful to forensic and police work as it helps facilitate recall from eyewitnesses. This highlights how the theory has a real-world practical application, i.e. it is useful outside the psychology laboratory.
However, the absence of cues cannot explain all kinds of forgetting.
For example, the interference theory of forgetting suggests that forgetting happens when memories interfere with and disturb one another; in other words, forgetting occurs when two pieces of information conflict.
Further, research evidence by McGeoch & McDonald (1931) showed that the more similar the two pieces of information, the stronger the interference effect. In other words, the more similar the information, the easier it is to forget. The theory may have overlooked the effect of interference on forgetting, and therefore it can be criticised as reductionist.
The Godden and Baddeley (1975) research lack ecological validity. Although this study was conducted in natural settings, the testing materials were artificial. The word list used in the study had no personal meaning to the participants and didn’t resemble actual memory usage in the real world.
Research evidence supporting cue-dependent forgetting is criticised for having the risk of circular reasoning.
Circular reasoning is a type of informal fallacy in which a conclusion is reached that is not materially different from something assumed as a premise of the argument.
In other words, the argument assumes what it is supposed to prove.
It is worth noting that no evidence has shown whether the cue has been encoded along with the testing material nor which test materials are successfully stored. The theory is difficult to refute because it does not explain whether recall failure occurs because the information is not stored or the right cue is not present.
Cue-dependent forgetting, also known as memory retrieval failure, is when an individual fails to recall a memory because cues/ stimuli that were present when the memory was encoded are not present.
No, cue-dependent theory explains forgetting in long-term memory.
Godden and Baddeley (1975) concluded that the mismatch between the external cues available at learning and recall led to retrieval failure.
Imagine someone is talking about a holiday you went on together, but you don't recall it. Suddenly they mention a restaurant, and you start to recollect memories from the holiday; this is an example of cue-dependent forgetting.
The cue dependency theory of forgetting is one of five forgetting theories in cognitive psychology.
How would you like to learn this content?
How would you like to learn this content?
Free psychology cheat sheet!
Everything you need to know on . A perfect summary so you can easily remember everything.
Be perfectly prepared on time with an individual plan.
Test your knowledge with gamified quizzes.
Create and find flashcards in record time.
Create beautiful notes faster than ever before.
Have all your study materials in one place.
Upload unlimited documents and save them online.
Identify your study strength and weaknesses.
Set individual study goals and earn points reaching them.
Stop procrastinating with our study reminders.
Earn points, unlock badges and level up while studying.
Create flashcards in notes completely automatically.
Create the most beautiful study materials using our templates.
Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.
Save explanations to your personalised space and access them anytime, anywhere!
Sign up with Email Sign up with AppleBy signing up, you agree to the Terms and Conditions and the Privacy Policy of StudySmarter.
Already have an account? Log in